Chat with us, powered by LiveChat For this discussion you must read the article on high-p request sequence. Define high-p request sequence. Have you ever tried high-p requests or do you think you would ever try high-p requ | Wridemy

For this discussion you must read the article on high-p request sequence. Define high-p request sequence. Have you ever tried high-p requests or do you think you would ever try high-p requ

For this discussion you must read the article on high-p request sequence. Define high-p request sequence. Have you ever tried high-p requests or do you think you would ever try high-p requests? Do you think this intervention would work for you to increase compliance among your young friends and/or relatives when you ask them to do things like picking up their toys, trash, etc.? If yes, why? If no, why?  

ARTICLE ATTATCHED 

100-200 WORDS. DUE 10/26/22 11:59 PM EST

THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCE AND RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT REINFORCER

DELIVERY ON CHILD COMPLIANCE

CHRYSTIN BULLOCK AND MATTHEW P. NORMAND

FLORIDA INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY

We compared the effects of a high-probability (high-p) instruction sequence and a fixed-time (FT) schedule of reinforcement on the compliance of 2 typically developing children. A multielement experimental design with a reversal component was implemented according to a multiple baseline across participants arrangement. Both the high-p and FT conditions resulted in increased compliance for both participants during the multielement sessions. These results suggest that it may be possible to increase compliance without a response requirement of the type arranged in the high-p instruction sequence.

DESCRIPTORS: behavioral momentum, compliance, high-probability instruction se- quence, fixed-time schedule

_______________________________________________________________________________

In 1988, Mace et al. reported a series of five experiments that evaluated the effects of a high probability (high-p) instruction sequence on compliance to low-probability (low-p) instruc- tions. A high-p instruction sequence involves the issuance of several instructions with which a participant is likely to comply immediately prior to the issuance of an instruction with which the participant is unlikely to comply. Praise or some other potential reinforcer is delivered following each instance of compli- ance. One major advantage of the high-p procedure is that physical contact with the learner is unnecessary. This is especially bene- ficial when working with individuals whose size or strength makes physical contact dangerous or in situations in which formal restrictions exist with respect to the use of physical contact. Mace et al. reported that the high-p instruction sequence improved compliance rates, latency to task initiation, and total task duration across

all five experiments, and subsequent research has mostly supported these findings (e.g., Mace & Belfiore, 1990).

However, it is possible that compliance to low-p instructions can be increased without a response requirement like the one in the high- p instruction sequence. Although Mace et al. (1988) did not report increased compliance following conditions in which positive state- ments were delivered independent of partici- pant responding, other researchers have re- ported such increases using similar procedures. For example, E. G. Carr, Newsom, and Binkoff (1976) reported increased compliance following the reading of humorous stories during therapy sessions, and Kennedy, Itkonen, and Lindquist (1995) increased compliance by making pleas- ant comments prior to the issuance of low-p instructions.

The present study was designed to assess whether compliance could be increased via the delivery of preferred stimuli prior to the issuance of a low-p instruction. In doing so, a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule of reinforcement (with no antecedent manipulation), a high-p instruction sequence, and a fixed-time (FT) schedule of reinforcement that signaled the onset of an FR 1 schedule of reinforcement for compliance were evaluated.

This paper is based on research submitted by the first author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the MS degree at the Florida Institute of Technology. We thank Eb Blakely for his helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Please address correspondence to Matthew Normand, School of Psychology, Florida Institute of Technology, 150 W. University Blvd., Melbourne, Florida 32901 (e- mail: [email protected]).

doi: 10.1901/jaba.2006.115-05

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2006, 39, 495–499 NUMBER 4 (WINTER 2006)

495

METHOD

Participants and Setting

Two typically developing boys, Joey (age 2 years) and Ashton (age 3 years), were selected based on their ability to follow vocal instruc- tions, parent permission, and their prebaseline responses to a variety of simple instructions. Each participant was observed to comply with at least 15 instructions that required a simple and discrete response and not to comply with at least five other similar instructions. All sessions took place in designated areas of the partici- pants’ homes. The experimenter, the partici- pant, and one observer were present during the experimental sessions.

Response Definitions and Measurement

The dependent measure was the percentage of low-p instructions with which each partici- pant complied (see Table 1). The low-p instructions were defined as instructions issued by the experimenter with which participants never complied during a 20-trial prebaseline assessment. This assessment involved presenting each instruction from a parent-generated list of potential low-p and high-p instructions a total of 20 times. The high-p instructions were those that resulted in compliance on every trial. Compliance was defined as the initiation of a response within 10 s of the stated instruction and total task completion within 60 s of task initiation. Percentage compliance was calculated by dividing the number of compliant responses

to low-p instructions by the total number of low-p instructions issued and multiplying by 100%.

Interobserver Agreement and Integrity of the Independent Variable

For 33% of all sessions, a second observer independently scored the percentage of compli- ance to low-p instructions. Interobserver agree- ment was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements for all trials and multiplying by 100%. Agreements were 100% across all conditions for both participants. In addition, an independent observer scored experimenter behavior for 33% of randomly selected sessions during the high-p instruction sequence and the FT components. Correct performance of the high-p instruction sequence required the de- livery of the reinforcer following each instance of compliance to a high-p instruction. For the FT condition, correct performance required the delivery of three reinforcers approximately once every 10 s. Integrity ratings were 100% across all conditions for both participants.

Experimental Design The experimental conditions were presented

according to a multielement design and were introduced across participants in a multiple baseline arrangement. The conditions were as follows: baseline; multielement, which consisted of (a) a baseline condition, (b) a high-p instruction sequence, and (c) an FT condition;

Table 1

High-p and Low-p Instructions Used for Each Participant

Participant Low p High p

Joey Put away your toy. Do this [clap hands, arms up, tap leg]. Come here. Touch [body part]. Put down the [item]. Give me [color]. Sit down. What color? What’s your name?

Ashton Take a bite of [food]. Do this [clap hands, arms up, tap leg]. Sit down. Touch [body part]. Go to your room. Give me [color].

What color? Give me five.

496 CHRYSTIN BULLOCK and MATTHEW P. NORMAND

and an FT condition. An extinction condition was included following the FT sessions, in which reinforcement was withheld for compli- ance; after this, the FT condition was reintro- duced.

Procedure

Stimulus preference assessment. Prior to the

start of baseline sessions, a brief stimulus

preference assessment (J. E. Carr, Nicolson, &

Higbee, 2000) was conducted to determine

a pool of potential edible reinforcers that could

be used. Edible items were selected because they

could be easily delivered and quickly consumed.

The use of edible items also made the removal

of a preferred item unnecessary, thereby in-

creasing the efficiency of the procedure and

minimizing the likelihood of problem behavior

resulting from the removal of a preferred item

or termination of a preferred activity. Each trial

of each condition began by asking the child to choose between two of the edible items identified as most preferred during the stimulus preference assessment.

Baseline. The experimenter was present in the room with the participant at a distance of approximately 1.5 m. A timer sounded every 30 s, prompting the experimenter to issue an instruction. The experimenter first established eye contact with the participant and then issued a low-p instruction. If the participant complied with the instruction, a small food item was delivered along with praise.

High-p instruction sequence. The high-p in- struction sequence involved the issuance of three high-p instructions (one every 10 s) followed by a low-p instruction approximately 10 s after the last high-p instruction. Compli- ance to each of the high-p instructions was followed by the delivery of a preferred edible item and praise. If the low-p instruction resulted in compliance, a preferred edible item was immediately delivered along with praise. If at any time the participant failed to comply with a high-p instruction, the trial was ended.

FT condition. An FT 10-s reinforcement schedule was arranged that was comparable to the average schedule of food delivery in the high-p condition. The completion of the FT schedule signaled the onset of an FR 1 schedule for compliance to the low-p instruction. The edible item was the same or similar to that which had been delivered in the momentum phase. The experimenter said ‘‘here,’’ ‘‘here you go,’’ or a similar phrase to gain the participant’s attention prior to delivering the preferred item and to counterbalance the attention given in issuing the high-p instructions in the high-p condition. A preferred edible item and praise followed each instance of compliance.

Extinction. This condition was identical to the baseline condition except that there were no programmed consequences for compliance.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Figure 1 depicts the percentage of compli- ance to low-p instructions for the 2 participants during each session across all phases of the study. For both participants, overall compliance to low-p instructions increased relative to the initial baseline performance in the FT compo- nent of the multielement condition and was maintained during the FT-alone condition. For Ashton, compliance to low-p instructions during the initial baseline sessions averaged 4%. During the multielement phase, his compliance averaged 58% during the baseline condition, 81% in the high-p condition, and 85% in the FT condition. When the FT condition was implemented alone, compliance to low-p instructions was 100%. During extinction, Ashton’s percentage of compliance fell to an average of 19%. When the FT condition was reinstated, Ashton’s percentage of compliance to low-p instructions was 100%.

Joey’s initial baseline averaged 5% compli- ance to the low-p instructions. During the multielement phase, the baseline condition averaged 15% compliance, the high-p condition averaged 40% compliance, and the FT condi-

COMPLIANCE IN CHILDREN 497

tion averaged 53% compliance. When the FT condition was implemented alone, compliance averaged 84%. During extinction, Joey’s overall compliance was 17%. After extinction, the FT condition was reintroduced, and compliance increased to 96%. Overall, the results suggest that the antecedent manipulations increased compliance to low-p instructions relative to initial baseline levels, although the FT condi- tion produced the greatest increase, and this increase persisted when the FT condition was implemented alone. Two potential explanations are suggested for this finding.

First, in both the high-p instruction sequence and the FT schedule, several potential re- inforcers are introduced immediately prior to the issuance of a low-p instruction. The increased presence of preferred items might act as an abolishing operation whereby the effectiveness of escape as a reinforcer is lessened and all behavior that has produced such escape in the past is abated (e.g., Lalli et al., 1999). Second, the frequent delivery of reinforcers by the experimenter in a common setting across all conditions could have increased the persistence

of compliance in the presence of the experi- menter (e.g., Nevin, 1996).

Although it appears that compliance can be increased without the response requirement of the high-p instruction sequence, a definitive conclusion cannot be drawn for two reasons. First, the multielement arrangement failed to yield clear discriminations among the experi- mental conditions. Second, increasing rein- forcement in the presence of a given stimulus should increase the resistance to change of any behavior that has been reinforced in the presence of that stimulus, even if reinforcement is independent of the behavior (Nevin, Tota, Torquato, & Shull, 1990). In the present study, all three reinforcement conditions occurred in the same context; this could have strengthened compliance in all three conditions, thereby producing the pattern of behavior obtained in the multielement phase.

Several other limitations of the current study should be noted. First, the preferred edible items and praise were delivered following all instances of compliance in all conditions. As such, repeated contact with these potential

Figure 1. Percentage of compliance with low-p instructions for Joey and Ashton during baseline, multielement, FT, extinction, and FT conditions.

498 CHRYSTIN BULLOCK and MATTHEW P. NORMAND

reinforcers could have produced the increase in compliance independent of the antecedent manipulations. However, because compliance failed to improve during the initial baseline conditions and did improve following the introduction of the antecedent manipulation conditions (including the FT condition follow- ing the extinction phase), it is likely that the antecedent manipulations were causally related to compliance.

Second, the low-p instructions involved simple and fairly discrete responses. It is possible that a procedure involving an FT schedule of reinforcement prior to the issuance of a low-p instruction would be ineffective for behaviors with a higher response requirement. For example, complying with the instruction ‘‘make your bed’’ involves the completion of multiple steps in a behavior chain and involves some amount of physical effort, whereas ‘‘tell me your name’’ requires a response involving fewer steps and little physical effort. Finally, in the extinction component the antecedent de- livery of preferred edible items and pro- grammed reinforcement for compliance were discontinued simultaneously, thereby weaken- ing experimental control.

Future research could address these limita- tions in several ways. The effects of the FT schedule could be evaluated in isolation so as to account for possible carryover effects among conditions arranged in a multielement format. Also, different physical settings could be used for the various experimental conditions to control for the increased rate of reinforcement

in similar settings across experimental condi- tions. Further analysis of the effects of anteced- ent manipulations (e.g., the high-p sequence and FT schedules) on more complex responses also is warranted.

REFERENCES

Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976). Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psychotic child. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- ogy, 4, 139–153.

Carr, J. E., Nicolson, A. C., & Higbee, T. S. (2000). Evaluation of a brief multiple-stimulus preference assessment in a naturalistic context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 353–357.

Kennedy, C. H., Itkonen, T., & Lindquist, K. (1995). Comparing interspersed requests and social comments as antecedents for increasing student compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 97–98.

Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C., Borrero, J., & Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition between positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–296.

Mace, F. C., & Belfiore, P. (1990). Behavioral momen- tum in the treatment of escape-motivated stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 507–514.

Mace, F. C., Hock, M. L., Lalli, J. S., West, B. J., Belfiore, P., & Pinter, P., et al. (1988). Behavioral momentum in the treatment of noncompliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 123–141.

Nevin, J. A. (1996). The momentum of compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 535–547.

Nevin, J. A., Tota, M. E., Torquato, R. D., & Shull, R. L. (1990). Alternative reinforcement increases resistance to change: Pavlovian or operant contingencies? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 359–379.

Received August 23, 2005 Final acceptance March 31, 2006 Action Editor, F. Charles Mace

COMPLIANCE IN CHILDREN 499

Our website has a team of professional writers who can help you write any of your homework. They will write your papers from scratch. We also have a team of editors just to make sure all papers are of HIGH QUALITY & PLAGIARISM FREE. To make an Order you only need to click Ask A Question and we will direct you to our Order Page at WriteDemy. Then fill Our Order Form with all your assignment instructions. Select your deadline and pay for your paper. You will get it few hours before your set deadline.

Fill in all the assignment paper details that are required in the order form with the standard information being the page count, deadline, academic level and type of paper. It is advisable to have this information at hand so that you can quickly fill in the necessary information needed in the form for the essay writer to be immediately assigned to your writing project. Make payment for the custom essay order to enable us to assign a suitable writer to your order. Payments are made through Paypal on a secured billing page. Finally, sit back and relax.

Do you need an answer to this or any other questions?

About Wridemy

We are a professional paper writing website. If you have searched a question and bumped into our website just know you are in the right place to get help in your coursework. We offer HIGH QUALITY & PLAGIARISM FREE Papers.

How It Works

To make an Order you only need to click on “Order Now” and we will direct you to our Order Page. Fill Our Order Form with all your assignment instructions. Select your deadline and pay for your paper. You will get it few hours before your set deadline.

Are there Discounts?

All new clients are eligible for 20% off in their first Order. Our payment method is safe and secure.

Hire a tutor today CLICK HERE to make your first order

Related Tags

Academic APA Writing College Course Discussion Management English Finance General Graduate History Information Justify Literature MLA